James P. Scanlan, Attorney at Law

Home Page

Curriculum Vitae

Publications

Published Articles

Conference Presentations

Working Papers

page1

Journal Comments

Truth in Justice Articles

Measurement Letters

Measuring Health Disp

Outline and Guide to MHD

Summary to MHD

Solutions

page3

Solutions Database

Irreducible Minimums

Pay for Performance

Between Group Variance

Concentration Index

Gini Coefficient

Reporting Heterogeneity

Cohort Considerations

Relative v Absolute Diff

Whitehall Studies

AHRQ's Vanderbilt Report

NHDR Measurement

NHDR Technical Issues

MHD A Articles

MHD B Conf Presentations

MHD D Journal Comments

Consensus/Non-Consensus

Spurious Contradictions

Institutional Corresp

page2

Scanlan's Rule

Outline and Guide to SR

Summary to SR

Bibliography

Semantic Issues

Employment Tests

Case Study

Case Study Answers

Case Study II

Subgroup Effects

Subgroup Effects NC

Illogical Premises

Illogical Premises II

Inevitable Interaction

Interactions by Age

Literacy Illustration

RERI

Feminization of Poverty S

Explanatory Theories

Mortality and Survival

Truncation Issues

Collected Illustrations

Income Illustrations

Framingham Illustrations

Life Table Illustrations

NHANES Illustrations

Mort/Surv Illustration

Credit Score Illustration

Intermediate Outcomes

Representational Disp

Statistical Signif SR

Comparing Averages

Meta-Analysis

Case Control Studies

Criminal Record Effects

Sears Case Illustration

Numeracy Illustration

Obesity Illusration

LIHTC Approval Disparitie

Recidivism Illustration

Consensus

Algorithm Fairness

Mortality and Survival 2

Mort/Survival Update

Measures of Association

Immunization Disparities

Race Health Initiative

Educational Disparities

Disparities by Subject

CUNY ISLG Eq Indicators

Harvard CRP NCLB Study

New York Proficiency Disp

Education Trust GC Study

Education Trust HA Study

AE Casey Profic Study

McKinsey Achiev Gap Study

California RICA

Nuclear Deterrence

Employment Discrimination

Job Segregation

Measuring Hiring Discr

Disparate Impact

Four-Fifths Rule

Less Discr Alt - Proc

Less Discr Altl - Subs

Fisher v. Transco Serv

Jones v. City of Boston

Bottom Line Issue

Lending Disparities

Inc & Cred Score Example

Disparities - High Income

Underadjustment Issues

Absolute Differences - L

Lathern v. NationsBank

US v. Countrywide

US v. Wells Fargo

Partial Picture Issues

Foreclosure Disparities

File Comparison Issues

FHA/VA Steering Study

CAP TARP Study

Disparities by Sector

Holder/Perez Letter

Federal Reserve Letter

Discipline Disparities

COPAA v. DeVos

Kerri K. V. California

Truancy Illustration

Disparate Treatment

Relative Absolute Diff

Offense Type Issues

Los Angeles SWPBS

Oakland Disparities

Richmond Disparities

Nashville Disparities

California Disparities

Denver Disparities

Colorado Disparities

Nor Carolina Disparitie

Aurora Disparities

Allegheny County Disp

Evansville Disparities

Maryland Disparities

St. Paul Disparities

Seattle Disparities

Minneapolis Disparities

Oregon Disparities

Beaverton Disparities

Montgomery County Disp

Henrico County Disparitie

Florida Disparities

Connecticut Disparities

Portland Disparities

Minnesota Disparities

Massachusetts Disparities

Rhode Island Disparities

South Bend Disparities

Utah Disparities

Loudoun Cty Disparities

Kern County Disparities

Milwaukee Disparities

Urbana Disparities

Illinois Disparities

Virginia Disparities

Behavior

Suburban Disparities

Preschool Disparities

Restraint Disparities

Disabilities - PL 108-446

Keep Kids in School Act

Gender Disparities

Ferguson Arrest Disp

NEPC Colorado Study

NEPC National Study

California Prison Pop

APA Zero Tolerance Study

Flawed Inferences - Disc

Oakland Agreement

DOE Equity Report

IDEA Data Center Guide

Duncan/Ali Letter

Crim Justice Disparities

U.S. Customs Search Disp

Deescalation Training

Career Criminal Study

Implicit Bias Training

Drawing Inferences

Diversion Programs

Minneapolis PD Investig

Offense Type Issues CJD

Innumerate Decree Monitor

Massachusetts CJ Disparit

Feminization of Poverty

Affirmative Action

Affirm Action for Women

Other Affirm Action

Justice John Paul Stevens

Statistical Reasoning

The Sears Case

Sears Case Documents

The AT&T Consent Decree

Cross v. ASPI

Vignettes

Times Higher Issues

Gender Diff in DADT Term

Adjustment Issues

Percentage Points

Odds Ratios

Statistical Signif Vig

Journalists & Statistics

Multiplication Definition

Prosecutorial Misconduct

Outline and Guide

Misconduct Summary

B1 Agent Cain Testimony

B1a Bev Wilsh Diversion

B2 Bk Entry re Cain Call

B3 John Mitchell Count

B3a Obscuring Msg Slips

B3b Missing Barksdale Int

B4 Park Towers

B5 Dean 1997 Motion

B6 Demery Testimony

B7 Sankin Receipts

B7a Sankin HBS App

B8 DOJ Complicity

B9 Doc Manager Complaints

B9a Fabricated Gov Exh 25

B11a DC Bar Complaint

Letters (Misconduct)

Links Page

Misconduct Profiles

Arlin M. Adams

Jo Ann Harris

Bruce C. Swartz

Swartz Addendum 2

Swartz Addendum 3

Swartz Addendum 4

Swartz Addendum 7

Robert E. O'Neill

O'Neill Addendum 7

Paula A. Sweeney

Robert J. Meyer

Lantos Hearings

Password Protected

OIC Doc Manager Material

DC Bar Materials

Temp Confidential

DV Issues

Indexes

Document Storage

Pre 1989

1989 - present

Presentations

Prosec Misc Docs

Prosec Misc Docs II

Profile PDFs

Misc Letters July 2008 on

Large Prosec Misc Docs

HUD Documents

Transcripts

Miscellaneous Documents

Unpublished Papers

Letters re MHD

Tables

MHD Comments

Figures

ASPI Documents

Web Page PDFs

Sears Documents

Pages Transfer


Innumerate Decree Monitors

(Sept. 24, 2021)

This page and its subpage principally address the mistaken belief that generally reducing adverse criminal justice outcomes (included adverse interactions with the police) will tend to reduce (a) relative racial differences in rates of experiencing the outcomes (as commonly presented in terms of the ratio of the Black rate to the white rate) and (b) the proportion Blacks make up of persons experiencing the outcomes (compared with the proportion Blacks make up of the population potentially experiencing the outcomes).  As I have explained in many places since 1996, the opposite is the case.

That is, as I have explained in scores of places with respect to any favorable or adverse outcome since 1987, when two groups differ in their susceptibility to an outcome, generally reducing the outcome, while tending to reduce relative differences in rates of avoiding the outcome (i.e., experiencing the opposite outcome), tends to increase relative difference in rates of experiencing the outcome itself.  Correspondingly, reducing the outcome, while tending to increase the proportion the more susceptible group makes up of persons avoiding the outcome (thus reducing all measures of difference between the proportion the group makes up of the population and the proportion it makes up of persons avoiding the outcome), tends also to increase the proportion the group makes up of persons experiencing the outcome itself (thus increasing all measures of difference between the proportion the group makes up of the population and the proportion it makes up of persons experiencing the outcome). 

But most people, including most statisticians and other data analysts, are unaware that it is even possible for the relative difference in a favorable outcome and the relative difference in the corresponding adverse outcome to change in opposite directions as the prevalence of an outcome changes, much less that such pattern tends to occur systematically.  And most persons who specialize in the analysis of demographic differences – whether regarding criminal justice, school discipline, borrowing, or voter requirement – mistakenly believe that reducing an adverse outcome will tend to reduce, rather than increase, (a) and (b) for the outcome. 

The failure to understand that it is even possible for the relative difference in a favorable outcome and the relative difference in the adverse outcome to change in opposite as the prevalence of the outcomes change is close to universal regardless of the type of outcome at issue.  In fact, so far as the published record reveals, no one has ever recognized such possibility save as a result of my work (though, inasmuch as the pattern is evident in so many kinds of data, I assume that some observers have recognized the pattern on their own even if such recognition as not been reflected in a published work).  The extent to which observers mistakenly believe that reducing an adverse outcome will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcome (as distinguished from simply having no view on the matter) varies.  In general, the more an observer specializes in the analysis of demographic differences – and the more an observer is exposed to discussion of patterns of demographic differences (which commonly reflect the mistaken view) – the more likely the observer is to believe that reducing an adverse outcome will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcome.

Criminal justice is an area where the mistaken belief that reducing adverse outcomes will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcomes is pervasive.  The fact that a person is a criminologist or studying criminology almost certainly means that the person mistakenly believe reducing adverse outcomes will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcomes. 

There exists a small industry comprised of expert monitors and persons seeking to be expert monitors of consent decrees arising from claims of discriminatory policing.  One can find online the applications of various entities seeking to monitor decrees in U.S. v. City of Ferguson and the U.S. v. Baltimore Police Department, both of which decrees are premised on the mistaken belief that generally reducing adverse criminal justice outcomes will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcomes.  The applications commonly emphasize the expertise the applicant entity can provide with respect to the analysis of data.  One can take for granted, however, that all the persons providing that expertise are unaware that generally reducing adverse criminal justice outcomes will tend to increase (a) and (b) for the outcomes and most or all mistakenly believe that reducing the outcomes will tend to reduce (a) and (b) for the outcomes.  Otherwise, one can expect the application to reflect the correct understanding.

The cost of the monitoring is not insubstantial.  The Venable applicant that was accepted for monitoring the Baltimore decree calls for a maximum yearly fee of 1.475million, which is apparently the average amount being charged.  In the case of the Ferguson decree, the application of the Patton Boggs firms that secured the monitor role indicates that the parties had agreed to a budget not to exceed $1.25 million over the first five years or $350,000 for any single year. I have not looked into what has actually been charged for the Ferguson monitoring.

My unsuccessful efforts to explain this issue to courts, parties, or monitors involved are reflected in Letter to United States Department of Justice and City of Ferguson, Missouri (Mar. 9, 2015), Submission re Ferguson Consent Decree (Apr. 11, 2016), Letter to the Honorable James K. Bredar (Feb. 14, 2017), Comments on (Baltimore) Consent Decree Monitor Selection (June 26, 2017), and Letter to Department of Justice (Apr. 13, 2017), and various emails to counsel or members of monitor teams.  Published articles regarding the mistaken understandings involving these two cases may be found in “Things DoJ doesn’t know about racial disparities in Ferguson,” The Hill (Feb. 22, 2016),  ), “Misunderstanding of Statistics Confounds Analyses of Criminal Justice Issues in Baltimore and Voter ID Issues in Texas and North Carolina,” Federalist Society Blog (Oct. 3, 2016), “Compliance Nightmare Looms for Baltimore Police Department,” Federalist Society Blog (Feb. 8, 2017), “Usual, But Wholly Misunderstood, Effects of Policies on Measures of Racial Disparity Now Being Seen in Ferguson and the UK and Soon to Be Seen in Baltimore,” Federalist Society Blog (Dec. 4, 2019), “The misunderstood effects of the Baltimore police consent decree,” The Daily Record (Feb. 15, 2018).

In the Ferguson case, the monitor is schedule to give status report that is viewable by the public on October 7, 2021, and comments regarding the matter may be submitted by October 5.  Time permitting, I may submit a comment requesting to address whether generally reducing adverse outcomes should reduce or increase (a) and (b) for the outcomes and to discuss available data showing what in fact has occurred.

It remains to be seen whether, in the event that data become available in either case that show that general reductions in adverse criminal justice outcomes were accompanied by increases in (a) and (b) for the outcomes, those increases will be discussed as something that occurred despite the general reductions or some that, contrary to the original expectations of the parties, the courts, and the monitors, the increase were something that numerate observers should have expected to happen.